ABSTRACT
Lots of research has been
done on attitude and personality variables and their ability to predict
success in English language learning, but very little has focused on the
specific stimulus of the test, probably the major stimulus for many of
our learners. In this study, I use test results and questionnaire data
to examine the relationship between attitudes and preferences in testing
and success on school exams. I also look at motivation: intrinsic, extrinsic,
long-term, and short. Most of our learners are test-motivated, but the
successful ones tend to be interested in English for reasons that are intrinsic
to English and not test-dependent: pleasure, fun, and career prospects.
In addition, the really successful ones would like to see writing and more
productive forms of evaluation brought in.
PRESENTER
BIOGRAPHY
Myung-sook Jung majored
in French and minored in English at Kyungpook National University, graduating
in 1985. She's been an English teacher for seven years and now works at
Ku-am Girls' Middle School in Taegu. She's interested in writing and teaching
higher level discourse skills, and also very interested in testing reform.
MATERIALS
RIght
Click Here to download the HWP version of these materials - complete
with beautiful pie charts and lovely formatting
INTRODUCTION: LEARNERS,
TEACHERS, TESTS
Because tests are thought
to guarantee a place in the
so-called good high schools,
and subsequently, good
universities and good jobs,
they are a big part of students'
lives and thus teachers'
lives. According to Seo et al,
students at Kwanchon Middle
School preferred tailor-made
materials developed specially
for them to the text in class,
but they were worried about
tests and therefore considered
the text-book more
"effective" (Seo et al, 1998: 51-52).
This creates a difficulty,
known as "washback", for
teachers who want to "negotiate"
the syllabus or use their
own materials. Martin points
out, in a study on writing in
American middle schools,
that learners do not see broader
learning aims and "passing
the exam" as connected in any
way (Martin 1983: 211).
Thus teachers can't help but
emphasize tests. But this
emphasis can cut short the aims
of education, focusing attention
prematurely on the result
rather than the process,
and frustrating those who make a
lot of effort.
A CASE IN POINT: MY STUDENT
SUNG-EUN
One example of this I have
personally observed was
my own student, Sung-eun,
who studied hard and steadily.
Although I gave her considerable
praise, she found that she
couldn't consistently get
good marks. After several tests, I
noticed her not concentrating
on her studying any more and
pointed out her idleness.
To my embarrassment, she burst
into tears, for her parents
had similarly scolded her with
similar results.
But perhaps the reason she
did badly lies in the way
she studies. And the way
she studies may well result from
the way she thinks about
tests. Perhaps Sung-eun is
trapped in a vicious circle:
short-term
motivation---intensive but
narrow and inefficient
studying---poor results---even
more intense and even
more short term motivation---another
bad
results---scolding from
outside---dislike of studying
English......etc.
CLASS DISCUSSION: FOR
LESS COMPETITIVE AND
MORE PRODUCTIVE FORMS
OF ASSESSMENT
Sung-eun was not alone in
her frustration. After
final-term of second semester,
I held a class discussion
about tests with my students.
A number of students made
it clear that they thought
the discrete point test was a far
from accurate measure of
productive ability. They argued
that it was not appropriate
to evaluate the students
according to indirect and
passive tests in English, or, for
that matter in music or
art.
Some students mentioned
that they preferred direct
performance measurement
in a pair-work or group-work
writing task to the discrete
point mid-term and final
examinations, which were
usually grammar-focused. For
example, during the class,
first, teachers would show
students the topic or situation,
about which the students
could think and then cooperate
to make good text. Group
work or pair work would
make the activity and the result
better organized.
Other learners expressed
reluctance to cooperate with
dull or different 'level'
students. They preferred to have
tests like writing by themselves
and
for themselves from
time to time during the
class under the topic which
teachers suggested previous
time. It emerged that they
thought writing was the
most adequate method to evaluate
their productive skill,
involving the recognition of
vocabulary or grammar and
moreover discourse organization
at a high level. Translation
was also suggested as a
possible test.
During this class discussion,
students suggested many
kinds of test format, but
they felt sorry that these formats
were not likely to adopted
in the real world. I wondered if
their suggestions and there
preferences were reflective of
underlying anxieties and
attitudes which might also be
reflected in their test
scores. So in addition to a number of
suggestions, I took away
from this discussion three
constructs I wanted to examine
in the light of test scores:
attitudes towards tests,
attitudes towards English study,
and writing proficiency.
TEST ANXIETY, LANGUAGE
ATTITUDE, AND
PRODUCTIVE SKILLS: A
BRIEF LOOK AT LITERATURE
As a practicing teacher doing
"action research", I was
unable to do a thorough
literature survey to prepare the
ground for the following
study. But I did notice that the
research on anxiety and
motivation has generally dealt with
the topic generally and
not focused on the specific stimulus
of testing. (Ellis 1994:
481-482, Chaudron 1988: 101-104).
Gardner and Lambert, for
example, took their chief
variables from the ESL situation
in Canada, distinguishing
between people who were
"instrumentally motivated" or
interested in assimilating
to the host culture. These are
clearly not appropriate
categories for analyzing the attitudes
and motivations of Korean
middle school learners in an EFL
situation (See Ellis 1994:
207-211). Dornyei and Csizer,
working in an EFL situation
in Hungary, give categories of
classroom motivation that
are much more appropriate to
Korea. They found that interim
goal setting by teachers for
learners was the most underutilized
strategy of all of their
motivational strategies.
(Dornyei and Csizer, 1998: 220).
Teachers often appear to
simply rely on mid-term and
end-term tests to motivate.
But test-based motivation
may be too extrinsic to
learners and too abstracted
from their day-to-day interests
to work consistently. Ellis
tells us:
"Whereas leaners' beliefs
about language learning are
likely to be fairly stable,
their affective states tend to be
volatile, affecting not
only overall progress but responses to
particular learning activities
on a day-by-day and even
moment-by-moment basis."
(Ellis 1994: 483)
This suggests, as my learners
suggested in our
discussion, a day-by-day
or even moment-by-moment form
of assessment--one that
is part of the process rather than
simply an evaluation of
the product. Chambers, examining
various classroom activities
which motivate or fail to
motivate learners, found
that many learners enjoyed project
work, pairwork, and groupwork,
although he also found that
these were rarely used (Chambers,
1998: 238). Similarly, a
recent study by Ko Kyounghee
showed that many learners
believe that less competitive
ways of developing and
assessing language skills
lesson emotional stress and may
well be helpful in, for
example, listening tests (Ko 1998:
22).
Here in Korea, there is
quite a bit of recent evidence
that our current forms of
assessment favor learners who
work by themselves over
those who enjoy group oriented
activities. Kim Jee-in,
working with elementary children,
found some evidence that
more introverted and intuitive
learners tended to do better
in English than outgoing,
sensitive ones (Kim 1998:
104), and Lee Eui-kap argues
that high school learners
who are introverted tend to do
better if their writing
work was analytically assessed,
rather than holistically
(Lee 1998: 122). We may be
unknowingly punishing a
particular kind of learning style,
and turning a valid attitude
to language learning into an
ineffective one.
In the following exploratory
study, I want to examine
the test scores in the light
of three constructs: test attitude,
language learning motivation,
and writing proficiency.
There are of course many
disadvantages to looking at
complex and very subjective
elements like attitude and
motivation, particularly
in a naturalistic classroom setting.
Even though there may be
lots of uncontrolled but
interesting variables in
getting good marks or bad marks on
the test (that is, the difference
of students' diligence,
hakwan attendance, teacher
enthusiasm, cheating......etc), I'll
neglect these factors because
it is beyond my ability to
control all of these things
and they are very difficult to
prove or measure. This,
of course, weakens the internal
validity of my study, but
it increases relevance to
classroom conditions, and
thus offers external validity.
In addition to examining
test attitude and study
motivation, I will examine
the relationship between test
scores and productive ability.
At school, we make students
have mostly reading and
listening mid-term and final-term
exams which, I think, are
not strongly based on the context
or real situation and have
limitations as measurements of
productive skill like writing
and speaking. Kellogg
demonstrated a very weak
relationship between exam scores
and productive ability in
English in a study among Korean
middle school learners in
1998, but his study was restricted
to a comparison of oral
output and test scores. Here, I will
extend Kellogg's finding
to written output (Kellogg 1998,
Kellogg 1999; Seo et al,
1998).
SEVEN HYPOTHESES
In order to look at the relationship
between test scores,
learner motivation, and
written output, I formulated the
following seven hypotheses,
which are provable and
disprovable through my data,
which consists of
questionnaire results, test
scores, and output on a writing
task.
H1. Students tend to have
test-centered motivation in
studying English.
H2. Learners who are high
scorers on listening and
reading tests tend to have
future oriented aims (career) or
pleasure and interest in
English. The students who don't
study English voluntarily,
that is, those who have
short-term motivation in
comparison with those who have
long-term motivation get
low scores on tests.
H3. Learners who are high
scorers on listening and
reading tests tend to prefer
discourse questions to grammar
questions or vocabulary
recognition questions.
H4. Low scoring students
on listening and reading tests
prefer vocabulary recognition
questions to grammar
questions or discourse questions.
H5. Students prefer different
test formats which need
productive skills to discrete
point tests like formal
mid-term or final-term exam.
H6. High scorers on the tests
prefer direct test such as
writing or translation to
indirect mid-term or final-term
exams.
H7. Passive skills do not
accurately predict actual
production. That is, learners
who score highly on listening
tests do not necessarily
do well on writing tasks.
SUBJECTS
The subjects were 42 girl
students of second grade
who are attending at Kwan-eum
middle school in Taegu.
They have been taught English
for one year by me. Some
of them have learned from
me since entering middle school.
QUESTIONNAIRE
I used a questionnaire to
gather data on attitudes and
motivation. gathered data
from one class which is typical of
classes of second grade
in Kwan-eum girls' middle school.
The questionnaire, which
was thoroughly explained before it
was administered, ran as
follows:
1. What kind of question
do you like in the discrete
point test?
¨Í discourse questions
¨Î grammar questions
¨Ï vocabulary recognition questions
I will show you an example
of these questions.
¢¿ Here is the
example of discourse question, which is
based on the discourse and
has coherence between the
sentences.
¢¿ Choose the
most suitable answer and fill in the
blank.
Jane and Mary were very
good friends. Usually they
did their homework and played
together after school. On the
weekend they often went
to the park near their houses and
had a good time. But now
they
. Their
parents don't know why,
and they worry about it.
¨ç don't even
talk to each other ¨è are close friends
¨é do their homework
¨ê want to study
more
¨ë like to go to
the park.
¢¿ This is an
example of grammar question, which is
focused on a grammar point.
¢¿ Choose the
clumsy one which has a different
meaning.
¨ç I am
surprised that he didn't come.
= I am surprised
at him not coming.
¨è I am
sure that you'll pass the exam.
= I am sure
to pass the exam.
¨é He couldn't
come because of the rain.
= He couldn't
come because it rained.
¨ê Let me
introduce you to my brother.
= Why don't
you meet my brother?
¨ë How about
taking a walk?
=Let's take
a walk.
¢¿ An example
of vocabulary recognition questions
¢¿ Choose one
which does not belong with the others.
¨ç father
¨è sister ¨é friend ¨ê
brother
¨ë mother
2. What makes you study English?
¨Í
influence (pressure) of teachers or parents
¨Î
fun and pleasure or interests
¨Ï
good test scores
¨Ð
future oriented aims
3. What kind of assessment
do you prefer? (These
choices are based on the
opinion drawn from the class
discussion. Therefore you
can make reference to the class
discussion.)
¨Í
discrete point test (mid-term or final-term
listening reading exams)
¨Î
group activities
¨Ï
interview
¨Ð
memorizing of contents in the textbooks
¨Ñ
translating
¨Ó
writing
TEST SCORES
The test scores consist of
the outcomes of their
mid-terms and final terms.
These are divided into written
tests (which, contrary to
their name, actually focus on the
reading skill) and listening
tests. The tests were written by
me. I didn't want
the students to memorize whole words or
grammar parts but check
how much they comprehend the
context. So the discrete
point tests have lots of discourse
questions.
WRITING TASK
In addition, I made the students
do a writing task,
which was written about
the plan of their winter vacation
for twenty minutes during
the class. Their writing tasks
were evaluated by four separate
teachers using criteria such
as structure, the number
of correct or comprehensible
sentences and the length
of whole writing task. Each
teacher gave one to five
points to each writing task. This
means that the highest score
on the writing taks is 20 and
the lowest is 4 points.
ANALYSIS AND DISCUSSION
Below, I will discuss my
results by presenting the data
in chart form against each
hypothesis and deciding whether
the hypothesis stands or
falls. Let me therefore explain the
conventions used in the
charts.
The scores given in the
charts are labelled as follows:
'oneltest' means mid-term
listening test scores of
second semester.
'twoltest' means final-term
listening test scores of
second semester.
'onewtest' means scores
of mid-term discrete point
written tests of second
semester, which are focused on
reading ability.
'twowtest' means scores
of final-term discrete point
written tests of second
semester, which are based on
reading skill.
'writing' means scores of
students' writing tasks
which were done about the
plan of their winter vacation
during the class for 20
minutes.
For ease of analysis,
I use pie charts, boxplots and
scatterplots, as well as
statistical tests (Pearson product
moment correlations).
In the box plot, the black
line indicates the mean. The
large shaded "box" holds
fifty percent of the scores nearest
the mean. The extended "arms"
of the box indicate the fifty
percent of the scores furthest
from the mean, and any
extreme outliers are given
as discrete points, with the
student number next to the
dot. At the bottom of each box,
the number of students in
the group is given; note that the
groups are very different
in size.
In each scatterplot, each
student score is plotted along
two axes, so that a strong
relationship causes the scores to
appear in a line, while
a weak relationship shows them
scattered in a "cloud".
The strength of the relationship is
also indicated by the Pearson
correlations in the tables that
follow hypothesis 7.
RESULTS BY HYPOTHESIS
H1.Students tend to
have test-centered motivation in
studying English.
Analysis of data relevant
to H1.
This confirms that test scores
are a big part of
students' lives. If we add
the negative motivation of
"pressure" (presumably pressure
to improve a test score),
we get a majority of the
students. This means that the
majority of our students
are extrinsically, not intrinsically,
motivated, and that motivation
is short-term, and not
long-term in nature.
H2. Learners who are
high scorers on listening and
reading tests tend to have
future oriented aims(career) or
pleasure and interest in
English; that is, they are
intrinsically and not extrinsically
motivated. The students
who don't study English
voluntarily, that is, those who
have extrinsic motivation,
tend to get low scores on tests.
This hypothesis is generally
confirmed by the data.
Although the mean for those
motivated by test scores is
only slightly lower than
that for those motivated by career
interest, the much smaller
group which is motivated by
intrinsic interest in English
has a far higher mean than the
small group negatively motivated
by external pressure.
<Boxplots of test scores
and motivation>
Analysis of data relevant
to H2
Interests and pleasure may
cause learners to pay more
attention to the class or
the teachers and to participate in
English related class activities
autonomously and creatively.
Conversely, we can predict
that students who study
English because of the pressure
from their parents, teachers
or others instead of inherent
interests or career aims don't
do well on the test. The
pressure may cause the learners to
dislike English and they
are not likely to pay attention to
English if they are not
in the presence of observers or
watchmen. It appears that
students who have intrinsic
motivation are likely to
do better on tests than those who
have extrinsic motivation.
This is a fairly consistent trend
across all of the tests,
reading and listening.
H3. Learners who are high
scorers on listening and
reading tests tend to prefer
discourse questions to grammar
questions or vocabulary
recognition questions.
This hypothesis is generally
confirmed, although there
does not appear to be much
difference in level between
learners who like questions
about "discourse" and those
who like questions about
"grammar", and the distinction,
which is a new one in testing
in Korea, may be unclear to
most students.
Boxplots of listening scores
and question style
preference (42 girl students)
Box plots of scores of discrete
point tests and the
preference of question style.
Analysis of data associated
with H3
In general, high scorers
on the tests appear comfortable
with sentences and situations,
and not simply words.
Students who prefer discourse
questions are likely to read
the text or books which
have large vocabulary, grammar
parts and discourse organization,
trying to figure out
context. Perhaps this makes
students have ability to guess
or comprehend. When they
contact and acquire lots of text
with pleasure, they are
likely to learn lots of language. As
a result, they can get good
scores on the tests.
H4. Low scoring students
on listening and reading tests
prefer vocabulary recognition
questions to grammar
questions or discourse questions.
This hypothesis is amply
confirmed by the boxplots
above.
Analysis of data relevant
to H4
Low scorers on the tests
appear to prefer vocabulary
recognition questions. We
can hypothesize that they are
uncomfortable with ambiguity,
text, links between the
words such as cohesion or
coherence, and they just put
their focus on small bits
of familiar language. Therefore,
they may learn only small
bits of language.
H5. Students prefer
different test formats which need
productive skills to discrete
point tests like formal
mid-term or final-term exam.
This hypothesis is
completely disconfirmed, as a look
at the pie chart will show.
Analysis of data related
to Hypothesis 5
During the class discussion,
most of the students
expressed skeptical attitude
to the traditional discrete point
tests as evaluation. However,
to my surprise, this pie chart
shows us that a large number
of students selected discrete
point tests among the
methods of evaluation recommended
by themselves. One obvious
explanation for this result is
that learners are profoundly
conservative--they want what
they are used to, or what
authority has suggested, at any
rate.
H6. High scorers on
the tests prefer direct test such as
writing or translation to
indirect discrete point mid-term or
final-term exams.
This hypothesis is strikingly
confirmed by the boxplot,
where those who preferred
translation and writing tests to
the discrete point exams
got far higher mean scores.
Boxplots of listening scores
and preference of test format
One class of second year
girls (42 learners)
Box plots of discrete
point test scores
and preference of test
style(42 learners)
Analysis of data associated
with Hypothesis 6
High scorers tend to prefer
writing or translating as
tests. There are some possible
explanations for this
inclination. Writing and
translating demand the ability to
deal with vocabulary, grammar
and discourse organization.
Students who know lots of
language may well feel
confident in writing and
translating, and choose test format
which make distinctive classification
between scores.
There was only one learner
who preferred group
activities, and it was a
relatively high scorer. This single
example does not of course
disprove the findings of Lee
and Kim that our current
assessment methods favor
introverted students.
H7. Passive skills
do not accurately predict actual
production. That is, learners
who score highly on listening
tests do not necessarily
do well on writing tasks.
This hypothesis was proven
so far as listening is
concerned. However, the
"written" tests, which were
actually discrete point
tests of reading, have a fairly strong
relationship with the score
on the writing task, as the
correlations and scatterplots
that follow will show.
Analysis of data relevant
to H7
These correlations and scatter
plots show us the
relatively strong relationship
of discrete point reading tests
and the integrative, productive
skill of writing. This is in
contrast to the findings
of Seo et al and Kellogg, who
found correlations of only
.468 to .603 between standardized
discrete point tests and
a very coarse measurement of
spoken output. All the correlations
are statistically
significant at the p <
.01 level.
It seems that students who
have the ability to produce
more language than others
can get good marks on the
reading test which have
vocabulary, grammar, discourse
questions. As we saw in
the scatter plots and the
correlation chart, we can
predict reading test scores with
writing scores. In addition,
writing high scorers are usually
listening high scorers because
writing needs usage of lots
of vocabulary, grammar and
also discourse organization
than other skills. Perhaps,
if the tests are well designed, it
is possible to take advantage
of discrete point tests instead
of writing in Korean schools
where writing is so neglected
because of classroom size
constraints.
However, it seems that it
is difficult to predict writing
scores with listening scores;
the correlations of listening
scores and writing task
scores are relatively weak. In fact,
the correlations are very
similar to those found by Seo et al
and Kellogg in 1998 and
1999 between listening tests and
oral production.
CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS
Naturally, there are lots
of uncontrolled variables at
work, as with any study
which uses actual learners in
classroom conditions. Nevertheless,
the factors we have
looked at--attitudes towards
tests, attitudes towards
English, and writing proficiency--do
account for some
interesting features of
the test results.
Learners who are high scorers
on tests tend to have
future oriented aims (career)
or fun as motivation in
studying in English. Intrinsic
motivation is apparently more
characteristic of high scorers
than extrinsic motivation. In
addition, learners who are
high scorers on written tests of
mid or final term tend to
prefer discourse questions or
grammar questions to vocabulary
recognition. When they
study English, they read
the text or books, trying to figure
out the context the books
give them rather than checking
and learning by heart only
the words they don't know.
They have fun in the process
of doing like this and they
can get ability to guess
and comprehend. When they like
lots of text, they are likely
to get lots of information and
learn a lots of language.
As a result, they can get good
scores on the test.
On the other hand, low scoring
students on tests prefer
vocabulary recognition questions
to discourse questions or
grammar questions. They
are uncomfortable with ambiguity,
texts and links between
the words such as cohesion or
coherence. They just put
focuses on small bits of familar
language. Therefore, they
learn small bits of language. It
makes the students get low
scores in the vicious circle. We
can assume that the ability
to deal with input affect output.
Before the survey, I expected
that a large number of
students wouldn't like to
do the discrete point test.
However, to my surprise,
the result was different. This
perhaps indicates that those
who don't feel confident in
English are worried about
the burden of a different
evaluation method.
I discovered that productive
writing tasks as an
evaluation method are relevant
to current testing methods.
Thus students who got high
scores on writing did well on
listening tests and discrete
point tests based on reading.
However, it's difficult
to predict that students who are
good at listening will do
well on reading or writing.
Perhaps, as the students
argued in our initial class
discussion, this is because
writing logically implies other
skills. Another explanation
is that writing is so neglected in
Korean schools that only
the best students or most
autonomous ones go on to
develop it.
In Korea, recently the circumstance
of evaluation is
likely to change into performance
assessment. It is desirable
trend but it may be possible
in small size class. And it has
lots of problems like objectivity
or burden to teachers who
are charge of large class
(over 40 students a class) and
have lots of classes (more
than 20 classes a week) and
non-teaching related jobs.
If the test questions are well
designed and consider various
possibilities and not simply
one possible right answer,
perhaps it will reflect students'
performance ability, as
the close relationship between the
test scores and writing
appears to imply.
But perhaps it is necessary
to take a more radical view
of the necessity of testing.
During this study, I have been
preoccupied with the thought
that the very emphasis on
testing may be cheating
our students of good test results. I
have found that students
who have interest in studying
English and deal with large
amount of English get good
result on the tests. If
English were not a subject of
entrance exams, students
would have less pressure in
English. They would feel
relaxation in dealing with large
amount of English and would
be able to approach to
English through reading
books, watching films, listening to
music, talking with friends
in English in stead of analyzing
of grammar points and memorizing
of vocabulary. And they
won't get afraid of making
contact with English or English
speakers.
Tests strongly affect
students' lives as well as
teachers'. We can't reproach
students just because they
don't do well on tests;
such reproach, and such pressure
may in itself cause them
to do badly. And after all, test
scores are a very small
part of English ability. Even the
lowest scorers can memorize
the whole lyric lines of their
favorite singers. Both Chambers
and Dornyei and Csizer
have pinpointed the teacher
as being the single most
important factor in learner
motivation. Thus it is the
teachers' role to make students
have interest in English and
do their best.
REFERENCES:
Chambers, G.N., "Pupils'
perceptions of the foreign
language learning experience",
Language Teaching Research,
Volume 2, Number 3, 1998,
pp. 231-261.
Dornyei, Zoltan and Kata
Csizer, "Ten commandments
for motivating language
learners: the results of an empirical
study", Language Teaching
Research, Volume 2, Number 3,
1998, pp. 203-231.
Chaudron, C., Second Language
Classrooms: Research
on teaching and learning,
Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge: 1988
Ellis, R., The Study of Second
Language Acquistion,
Oxford University Press,
Oxford: 1994
Kellogg, David, "Pairs work:
a comparison of student
output in pairwork and teacher-fronted
interviews", West
Taegu English Language Teaching
Research Association,
Taegu: August 1998.
Kellogg, David, "HOGS and
LOGS: Developing
materials for different
levels of output generation",
unpublished manuscript,
1999.
Kim Jee-in, "Personality
Variables and EFL
Proficiency", English Teaching,
Vol. 53, No. 4, 1998, pp.
93-106.
Ko Kyounghee, "Cooperative
Learning in Noninteractive
Listening Tasks", English
Teaching, Vol. 53, No. 4, 1998,
pp. 3-30.
Lee Eui-kap, "The Effect
of Temperament Types,
Feedback Types, and Writing
Modes on EFL Students'
Task-based Writing", English
Teaching, Vol. 53, No. 4,
1998, pp. 107-134.
Martin, N., "Scope for intentions",
in Learning to write:
first language/second language,
Freedman & Pringle eds.,
Longman, Harlow: 1983
Seo et al, "Pairwork Áß
½É ÀÇ ¼ö ÁØ º°
±³ Áµ °³ ¹ß ¹×
Àû
¿ë", West Taegu
English Language Teaching Research
Association, Taegu: December
1998.
|